* Is Grammar obsolete?

Here’s an article by Scheffler, which I’ve edited a bit. It’s a free download from the Applied Linguistics Journal website. I have to say that I don’t think Scheffler gives an accurate account of SLA research, or of its practioners’ concerns or responsibilities. I could easily point to counter-studies and give counter arguments, but, anyway, I think it makes an interesting, if over-simplified, case.

When SLA research started, in the 1950s, it was seen by some as the death of ‘neanderthal teaching practices’ (Long, 1988). Teachers were expected to stop providing learners with explicit explanations of how a FL works and instead convert to ‘focus on form’, that is comprehensible input, negotiation for meaning, and implicit feedback.

But, mostly, this didn’t happen. Many teachers kept teaching to the structural syllabus, taught grammar explicitly, and used their learners’ L1 to make meaning clear. Skehan (1998: 94) suggested that teachers were ‘a conservative profession, out of touch with language acquisition studies’. Hence, basically, teachers stuck to methods that were familiar and easy to apply, and they were unwilling to become engaged with SLA research.

But the suggestion that these teachers teach the way they do because they are lazy and unprofessional is challenged by many. As, for example, Swan (2005: 386–387) and Butzkamm and Caldwell (2009: 242) point out, the history of FL teaching is not a ‘succession of failed methods’. On the contrary, countless competent FL users have been produced by traditional teaching, which means that the techniques condemned by SLA specialists seem to have worked for many learners and teachers.

So how is it possible that teaching procedures which do not fit an established SLA theory are, at least in some contexts, effective? Perhaps Long and Robinson (1998) are right when they say that successful learners learn in spite of them rather than because of them? We can only answer this question if we subject these procedures to rigorous empirical scrutiny and compare their effectiveness with the effectiveness of the techniques advocated on the basis of the latest SLA research findings. An important goal of SLA theory is to explain how learning is accomplished through teaching. No teaching procedure claimed by teachers to be effective should be disregarded, even if the focus of current theory is on something completely different.

As evidenced by recent surveys of the field (e.g. Ellis 2008), the amount of research that has been done in SLA in the past few decades is impressive. Comparative work on explicit and implicit instruction has also been undertaken (Norris and Ortega 2000). Yet, some of the more traditional techniques used in teaching (e.g. translation) have attracted surprisingly little attention of SLA researchers (Butzkamm and Caldwell 2009; Cook 2010). In addition, as Sheen (2005: 286) and Pawlak (2007: 170) say, there is a dearth of comparative research directly involving focus on forms and focus on form in real classrooms. However, when these two instructional approaches are researched and compared, it turns out that the former is more effective than the latter. Furthermore, there is some evidence that explicit teaching can actually affect learners’ performance in unplanned language use. Let us very briefly consider three relevant examples.

The first study is Housen et al. (2005). It is not a comparative study of the two types of focus, but it produced one finding relevant to the present discussion: explicit grammar instruction concerning French passive sentences and negation leads to improvements in unplanned oral FL performance. Secondly, Sheen (2005) demonstrates that pupils explicitly taught the rules of English question formation and adverb placement in addition to communicative activities do significantly better with respect to these features in oral interviews than those who are just involved in communication supported by corrective feedback. Thirdly, Pawlak (2007) shows that pupils subjected to the traditional presentation-practice-production type of instruction in English conditional sentences exhibit significant gains on a meaning-oriented dictogloss task.

It seems, then, that providing explicit grammar explanations to learners may not be a ‘neanderthal’ teaching practice after all. Furthermore, there is growing empirical evidence that contrastive L1–L2 explicit information may be necessary if FL learners are to master certain difficult L2 structures. One study from the late 1990s and one recent study will serve as examples.

Kupferberg (1999) examined the acquisition of the perfect aspect by advanced Hebrew university learners of English. English and Hebrew differ in their marking of aspect: in the former, a verbal complex with an auxiliary verb may be used, whereas in the latter lexical means are employed. This difference caused difficulty for the subjects in the study: they were able to recognize the past perfect construction in an English text, but generally avoided it in production. To test the effect of contrastive metalinguistic input (CMI) on production, the subjects were divided into two groups: the experimental group, which received CMI, and the control group, which was provided with L2 focused explicit instruction. The effects of the treatments were measured by means of written storytelling tasks, which created natural contexts for the use of the past perfect. Both groups exhibited significant gains in the number of the past perfect forms produced, but the gains of the experimental group were significantly higher that those of the control group.

Ammar et al. (2010) studied French learners of English in a communicative program at an elementary school in Canada. The research questions concerned the pupils’ ability to judge and form questions in English, their awareness of differences between French and English grammar in this area, and the relationship between the pupils’ performance and awareness. The authors used three tasks: a grammaticality judgement task, a scrambled questions task, and an interview. The first two tasks provided input for the interview, which was supposed to provide the researchers with data concerning the pupils’ explicit knowledge about question formation in French and English.

The findings were as follows: first, the pupils’ judgements of grammaticality and the questions that they formed revealed their reliance on rules of French grammar. Secondly, few learners were able to verbalize the rules, and thirdly, there was a positive correlation between making correct judgements and forming correct questions and awareness of L1–L2 contrasts. The authors offer the following conclusion concerning the usefulness of contrastive L1–L2 information: ‘it would appear that there is crucial information students need to know and that this information may best be provided through explicit instruction’(pp. 142–3).

If there was anything more ‘neanderthalian’ than explicit explanations of discrete grammar points, then it must have been translation. Since the Reform Movement in the second half of the 19th century, translation as a means of making meaning clear has been discouraged, and translation of disconnected sentences and texts has been generally condemned by FL teaching specialists (cf. Fotos 2005). It has also been ignored by most SLA researchers (Cook 2010: 21). There are signs, though, that even this long-time exile from SLA research may be finally allowed to make a comeback. The books published recently by Butzkamm and Caldwell (2009) and Cook (2010) are indications that translation is a topic worth researching and that teachers who resisted the ban on its use in the classroom may have known what they were doing. A very
recent report from one of such teachers is Kim’s (2011) study. Frustrated by her college students’ inability to improve the quality of their writing during the drafting process, Kim decided to ask them to translate their compositions into their native language, that is Korean. The translation exercise, with which they were familiar from their high school instruction, proved to be an eye opener for them: they were able to see the problems they had both in terms of form and content. Kim (2011: 160) concludes ‘that an approach based on grammar translation can be a useful means to achieve a communicative goal’.

SLA research is often undertaken with a view to improving the quality of FL teaching. As Butzkamm and Caldwell (2009: 242) observe, however, it is often undertaken by people ‘who have never been full time teachers in public schools and taught across the ability range for at least a couple of years’. Furthermore, it is undertaken by people who are often very dismissive of the opinions of those who on the basis of their experience maintain that ‘neanderthal teaching’ often actually works. ‘Let us try to understand not only what made it fail in some classrooms but also what made it tick in other contexts, and we’ll make some progress’ (Butzkamm and Caldwell 2009: 242).

Researchers in any area should not only remember that their theories may one day pass but should also be prepared to forget things they are surest of, as Jean Rostand (1962: 88) also said.

For references, see the article Scheffler,F. (2012) Theories Pass: Learners and Teachers Remain. Applied Linguistics, 33/5: 603–607.

Advertisements

One thought on “* Is Grammar obsolete?

  1. Thanks for the post. I think it should be questioned why presenting and encouraging methods of doing reliable researches in SLA are not included in current teacher training courses or EFL teacher programmes at universities. We’re about to land on Mars in a few decades even though we haven’t figured out how we learn another language.

    Like

Leave a Reply

Fill in your details below or click an icon to log in:

WordPress.com Logo

You are commenting using your WordPress.com account. Log Out / Change )

Twitter picture

You are commenting using your Twitter account. Log Out / Change )

Facebook photo

You are commenting using your Facebook account. Log Out / Change )

Google+ photo

You are commenting using your Google+ account. Log Out / Change )

Connecting to %s